Reading on Jan. 1, 2011:
The U-bend of Life
Ask people how they feel about getting older, and
they will probably reply in the same vein 心情 as Maurice Chevalier: “Old age isn’t so bad when
you consider the alternative.” Stiffening joints, weakening
muscles, fading eyesight and the clouding of memory, coupled with
the modern world’s careless contempt 轻视 for the old, seem a fearful prospect—better than
death, perhaps, but not much. Yet mankind is wrong to dread ageing.
Life is not a long slow decline from sunlit uplands towards
the valley of death. It is, rather, a U-bend.
When people start out on adult life, they are, on average, pretty
cheerful. Things go downhill from youth to middle age until they
reach a nadir /ˈneɪdɪə(r)/ 天底 commonly known as the mid-life
crisis. So far, so familiar. The surprising part happens after
that. Although as people move towards old age they lose things they
treasure—vitality, mental sharpness and looks—they also gain what
people spend their lives pursuing: happiness.
This curious finding has emerged from a new branch of economics
that seeks a more satisfactory measure than money of human
well-being. Conventional 常规的
economics uses money as a proxy 代表 for utility—the dismal [ˈdizməl]
令人忧郁的 way in which the
discipline talks about happiness. But some economists, unconvinced
that there is a direct relationship between money and well-being,
have decided to go to the nub (问题﹑事情等的)中心,要点 of the matter and measure happiness
itself.
Comparing countries: The rich, the poor and Bulgaria
These ideas have penetrated the policy arena, starting in Bhutan不丹,
where the concept of Gross National Happiness shapes the planning
process. All new policies have to have a GNH assessment, similar to
the environmental-impact assessment common in other countries. In
2008 France’s president, Nicolas Sarkozy, asked two
Nobel-prize-winning economists, Amartya Sen and Joseph Stiglitz, to
come up with a broader measure of national contentedness than GDP.
Then last month, in a touchy-feely 外露的 /人际互动 gesture not typical of
Britain, David Cameron announced that the British government would
start collecting figures on well-being.
There are already a lot of data on the subject collected by, for
instance, America’s General Social Survey, Eurobarometer and
Gallup. Surveys ask two main sorts of question. One concerns
people’s assessment of their lives, and the other how they feel at
any particular time. The first goes along the lines of: thinking
about your life as a whole, how do you feel? The second is
something like: yesterday, did you feel
happy/contented/angry/anxious? The first sort of question is said
to measure global well-being, and the second
hedonic快乐的 or
emotional well-being. They do not always
elicit [iˈlisit]
诱出,探出 the same response: having
children, for instance, tends to make people feel better about
their life as a whole, but also increases the chance that they felt
angry or anxious yesterday.
Statisticians trawl
[trɔ:l] through 查阅 the vast quantities of data these
surveys produce rather as miners panning 用淘金盘淘洗砂土 for gold. They are
trying to find the answer to the
perennial [pəˈreniəl]
长久的 question: what makes people
happy?
Four main factors, it seems: gender, personality, external
circumstances and age. Women, by and large, are slightly happier
than men. But they are also more
susceptible [səˈseptəbəl]
易受影响的 to depression: a fifth to
a quarter of women experience depression at some point in their
lives, compared with around a tenth of men. Which suggests either
that women are more likely to experience more extreme emotions, or
that a few women are more miserable than men, while most are more
cheerful.
Two personality traits shine through the complexity of
economists’ regression复原
analyses: neuroticism [njuə'rɔtəsizəm]
神经过敏症 and
extroversion‘心理’外向性. Neurotic
people—those who are prone to 易於某事物guilt, anger and anxiety—tend to be unhappy. This
is more than a tautological [ˌtɔ:təˈlɔdʒikəl]
冗赘的 observation about people’s
mood when asked about their feelings by pollsters or economists.
Studies following people over many years have shown that
neuroticism is a stable personality trait and a good
predictor of levels of happiness. Neurotic people are not just
prone to negative feelings: they also tend to have low emotional
intelligence, which makes them bad at forming or managing
relationships, and that in turn makes them unhappy.
Whereas neuroticism tends to make for gloomy types, extroversion
does the opposite. Those who like working in teams and who relish
享受 parties tend to be happier
than those who shut their office doors in the daytime and hole up
躲藏 at home in the evenings.
This personality trait may help explain some cross-cultural
differences: a study comparing similar groups of British, Chinese
and Japanese people found that the British were, on average, both
more extrovert and happier than the Chinese and
Japanese.
Then there is the role of circumstance. All
sorts of things in people’s lives, such as relationships,
education, income and health, shape the way they feel. Being
married gives people a considerable uplift, but not as big as the
gloom that springs from being unemployed. In America, being black
used to be associated with lower levels of happiness—though the
most recent figures suggest that being black or Hispanic is
nowadays associated with greater happiness. People with children in
the house are less happy than those without. More educated people
are happier, but that effect disappears once income is controlled
for. Education, in other words, seems to make people happy because
it makes them richer. And richer people are happier than poor
ones—though just how much is a source of argument.
The view from winter
Lastly, there is age. Ask a bunch of 30-year-olds and another of
70-year-olds (as Peter Ubel, of the Sanford School of Public Policy
at Duke University, did with two colleagues, Heather Lacey and
Dylan Smith, in 2006) which group they think is likely to be
happier, and both lots point to the 30-year-olds. Ask them to rate
their own well-being, and the 70-year-olds are the happier bunch.
The academics quoted lyrics written by Pete Townshend of The Who
when he was 20: “Things they do look awful cold / Hope I die before
I get old”. They pointed out that Mr Townshend, having passed his
60th birthday, was writing a blog that glowed
[gləu] 激情洋溢 with good humour.
Mr
Townshend may have thought of himself as a youthful radical, but
this view is ancient and conventional. The “seven ages of man”—the
dominant image of the life-course in the 16th and 17th
centuries—was almost invariably conceived 设想 as a rise in stature
[ˈstætʃə] 高境界,高水平,非凡的气质;身高(材) and contentedness to middle
age, followed by a sharp decline towards the grave. Inverting the
rise and fall is a recent idea. “A few of us noticed the U-bend in
the early 1990s,” says Andrew Oswald, professor of economics at
Warwick Business School. “We ran a conference about it, but nobody
came.”
People are least happy in their 40s and early 50s. They reach a
nadir at a global average of 46. Since then, interest in the U-bend
has been growing. Its effect on happiness is significant—about half
as much, from the nadir of middle age to the elderly peak, as that
of unemployment. It appears all over the world. David Blanchflower,
professor of economics at Dartmouth College, and Mr Oswald looked
at the figures for 72 countries. The nadir varies among
countries—Ukrainians, at the top of the range, are at their most
miserable at 62, and Swiss, at the bottom, at 35—but in the great
majority of countries people are at their unhappiest in their 40s
and early 50s. The global average is 46.
The U-bend shows up in studies not just of global well-being but
also of hedonic 快乐的 or
emotional well-being. One paper, published this year by Arthur
Stone, Joseph Schwartz and Joan Broderick of Stony Brook
University, and Angus Deaton of Princeton, breaks well-being down
into positive and negative feelings and looks at how the experience
of those emotions varies through life. Enjoyment and happiness dip
in middle age, then pick up; stress rises during the early 20s,
then falls sharply; worry peaks in middle age, and falls sharply
thereafter; anger declines throughout life; sadness rises slightly
in middle age, and falls thereafter.
Turn the question upside down, and the pattern still appears. When
the British Labour Force Survey asks people whether they are
depressed, the U-bend becomes an arc, peaking at 46.
Happier, no matter what
There is always a possibility that variations are the result not of
changes during the life-course, but of differences between cohorts
/ˈkəuhɔːt/一群人. A 70-year-old European may feel different to
a 30-year-old not because he is older, but because he grew up
during the second world war and was thus formed by different
experiences. But the accumulation of data undermines 侵蚀…的基础 the idea of a cohort effect.
Americans and Zimbabweans have not been formed by similar
experiences, yet the U-bend appears in both their countries. And if
a cohort effect were responsible, the U-bend would not show up
consistently in 40 years’ worth of data.
Another possible explanation is that unhappy people die early. It
is hard to establish whether that is true or not; but, given that
death in middle age is fairly rare, it would explain only a little
of the phenomenon. Perhaps the U-bend is merely an expression of
the effect of external circumstances. After all, common factors
affect people at different stages of the life-cycle. People in
their 40s, for instance, often have teenage children. Could the
misery of the middle-aged be the consequence of sharing space with
angry adolescents? And older people tend to be richer. Could
their relative contentment be the result of their piles of
cash?
The answer, it turns out, is no: control for cash, employment
status and children, and the U-bend is still there. So the growing
happiness that follows middle-aged misery must be the result not of
external circumstances but of internal changes.
People, studies show, behave differently at different ages. Older
people have fewer rows 争吵 and
come up with better solutions to conflict. They are better at
controlling their emotions, better at accepting misfortune and less
prone to anger. In one study, for instance, subjects were asked to
listen to recordings of people supposedly saying disparaging
贬低 things about them. Older
and younger people were similarly saddened, but older people less
angry and less inclined to pass judgment, taking the view, as one
put it, that “you can’t please all the people all the
time.”
There are various
theories as to why this might be so. Laura Carstensen, professor of
psychology at Stanford University, talks of “the uniquely human
ability to recognise our own mortality and monitor our own time
horizons”. Because the old know they are closer to death, she
argues, they grow better at living for the present活在当下. They come
to focus on things that matter now—such as feelings—and less on
long-term goals. “When young people look at older people, they
think how terrifying it must be to be nearing the end of your life.
But older people know what matters most.” For instance, she says,
“young people will go to cocktail parties because they might meet
somebody who will be useful to them in the future, even though
nobody I know actually likes going to cocktail parties.”
Death of ambition, birth of acceptance
There are other possible explanations. Maybe the sight of
contemporaries keeling over 倾覆 infuses survivors with a determination to make the
most of their remaining years. Maybe people come to accept their
strengths and weaknesses, give up hoping to become chief executive
or have a picture shown in the Royal Academy, and learn to be
satisfied as assistant branch manager, with their watercolour on
display at the church fete义卖会. “Being an old maid”, says one of the characters in
a story by Edna Ferber, an (unmarried) American novelist, was “like
death by drowning—a really delightful sensation 感觉 when you ceased struggling.” Perhaps
acceptance of ageing itself is a source of relief. “How pleasant is
the day”, observed William James, an American philosopher, “when we
give up striving to be young—or slender.”
Whatever the causes of the U-bend, it has consequences beyond the
emotional. Happiness doesn’t just make people happy—it also makes
them healthier. John Weinman, professor of psychiatry at
King’s College London, monitored the stress levels of a group of
volunteers and then inflicted 把…强加给 small wounds on them. The wounds of the least
stressed healed twice as fast as those of the most stressed. At
Carnegie Mellon University in Pittsburgh, Sheldon Cohen infected
people with cold and flu viruses. He found that happier types were
less likely to catch the virus, and showed fewer symptoms of
illness when they did. So although old people tend to be less
healthy than younger ones, their cheerfulness may help counteract
their crumbliness破碎性.
Happier people are more productive, too. Mr Oswald and two
colleagues, Eugenio Proto and Daniel Sgroi, cheered up a bunch of
volunteers by showing them a funny film, then set them mental tests
and compared their performance to groups that had seen a neutral
film, or no film at all. The ones who had seen the funny film
performed 12% better. This leads to two conclusions. First, if you
are going to volunteer for a study, choose the economists’
experiment rather than the psychologists’ or psychiatrists’.
Second, the cheerfulness of the old should help counteract their
loss of productivity through declining cognitive skills—a point
worth remembering as the world works out how to deal with an ageing
workforce.
The ageing of the rich world is normally seen as a burden on the
economy and a problem to be solved. The U-bend argues for a more
positive view of the matter. The greyer the world gets, the
brighter it becomes—a prospect which should be especially
encouraging to Economist readers (average age 47).
加载中,请稍候......