标签:
成人依恋理论心理学 |
John Bowlby - Attachment
Theory(依恋理论)
Attachment is a special emotional relationship that involves an
exchange of comfort, care, and pleasure. The roots of research on
attachment began with Freud(弗洛伊德(Sigmund,
1856-1939,
奥地利神经学家、精神病医学家、精神分析的创始人)’s theories
about love, but another researcher is usually credited as the
father of attachment theory. John Bowlby(波尔比(1907—1990),英国心理学家,杰出的儿童精神病学家。他将心理分析、认知心理学和进化生物学等学科统合在一起,纠正了弗洛伊德精神分析理)
devoted extensive research to the concept of attachment, describing
it as a “…lasting psychological connectedness between human
beings" (Bowlby, 1969, p. 194). Bowlby shared the psychoanalytic
view that early experiences in childhood have an important
influence on development and behavior later in life. Our early
attachment styles are established in childhood through the
infant/caregiver relationship. In addition to this, Bowlby believed
that attachment had an evolutionary component; it aids in survival.
“The propensity to make strong emotional bonds to particular
individuals [is] a basic component of human nature” (Bowlby, 1988,
3).
Bowlby believed that there are four distinguishing characteristics
of attachment:
- Proximity Maintenance(接近性维持) – The desire to be near the people we are attached to.
- Safe Haven(安全港) – Returning to the attachment figure for comfort and safety in the face of a fear or threat.
- Secure Base(安全基础) – The attachment figure acts as a base of security from which the child can explore the surrounding environment.
- Separation Distress(分离忧虑) – Anxiety that occurs in the absence of the attachment figure.
Ainsworth(安斯沃斯(1913-1999),美国心理学家,对心理学的最重要的贡献是早期情感依恋方面的研究。1989年获美国心理学会颁发的杰出科学贡献奖。)’s
“Strange Situation" Assessment
Parent and infant are along in child explores without partntal participation. stranger enters room, talks to parent, and approaches infant. parent leaves quietly. parent returns and comforts infant. |
During the 1970’s, researcher Mary Ainsworth further expanded
upon Bowlby’s groundbreaking work in her now-famous “Strange
Situation” study. The study involved observing children between
the ages of 12 to 18 months responding to a situation in which they
were briefly left alone and then reunited with their mother
(Ainsworth, 1978).
Based on these observations, Ainsworth concluded that there were
three major styles of attachment: secure attachment, ambivalent insecure attachment(矛盾性不安全依恋),
and avoidant-insecure
attachment(逃避性不安全依恋). Researchers Main and Solomon
(1986) added a fourth attachment style known as disorganized-insecure
attachment(错乱性不安全依恋). Numerous studies have
supported Ainsworth’s conclusions and additional research has
revealed that these early attachment styles can help predict
behaviors later in life.
Attachment Through Life
Before you start blaming relationship problems on your parents, it
is important to note that attachment styles formed in infancy are
not necessarily identical to those demonstrated in adult
romantic-attachment. A great deal of time has elapsed between
infancy and adulthood, so intervening experiences also play a large
role in adult attachment styles. Those described as ambivalent or
avoidant in infancy can become securely attached as adults, while
those with a secure attachment in childhood can show insecure
attachment styles in adulthood. Basic temperament(性格) is also thought to play a
partial role in attachment.
But research in this area does indicate that patterns established
in childhood have an important impact on later relationships.
Researchers Hazen and Shaver (1987) found a number of different
beliefs about relationships amongst adults with differing
attachment styles. Securely attached adults tend to believe that
romantic love is enduring. Ambivalently attached adults report
falling in love often, while those with avoidant attachment styles
describe love as rare and temporary.
While we cannot say that infant attachment styles are identical to
adult romantic-attachment styles, research has shown that early
attachment styles can help predict patterns of behavior in
adulthood.
Secure Attachment
characteristics of secure attachment | |
as children | as adults |
separate essily from parent | have trusting,lasting relationships |
seek comfort from parent when frightened | tend to have good selfesteem |
greet parents positively | comfortable sharing feelings |
prefers parent to stranger. | seeks out social support. |
Children who are securely attached do not experience significant
distress when separated from caregivers. When frightened, these
children will seek comfort from the parent or caregiver. Contact
initiated by a parent is readily accepted by securely attached
children and they greet the return of a parent with positive
behavior. While these children do not become exceptionally
distressed by a parent’s absence, they clearly prefer parents to
strangers. Parents of securely attached children tend to play more
with their children. Additionally, these parents react more quickly
to their children’s needs and are generally more responsive to
their children than the parents of insecurely attached children.
Studies have shown that securely attached children are more
empathetic during later stages of childhood. These children are
also described as less disruptive, less aggressive, and more mature
than children with ambivalent or avoidant attachment styles. As
adults, those who are securely attached tend to have trusting,
long-term relationships. Other key characteristics of securely
attached individuals include having high self-esteem, enjoying
intimate relationships, seeking out social support, and an ability
to share feelings with other people. In one study, researchers
found that women with a secure attachment style had more positive
feelings about their adult romantic relationships than other women
with insecure attachment styles (Mccarthy G., 1999).
Ambivalent Attachment
Children who are ambivalently attached tend to be extremely
suspicious of strangers. These children display considerable
distress when separated from a parent or caregiver, but do not seem
reassured or comforted by the return of the parent. In some cases,
the child might passively reject the parent by refusing comfort, or
may openly display direct aggression toward the parent. According
to Cassidy and Berlin (1994), ambivalent attachment is relatively
uncommon, with only 7% to 15% of infants in the United States
displaying this attachment style. In a review of ambivalent
attachment literature, Cassidy and Berlin also found that
observational research consistently links ambivalent-insecure
attachment to low maternal availability. As these children grow
older, teachers often describe them as clingy and over-dependent.
As adults, those with an ambivalent attachment style often feel
reluctant about becoming close to others and worry that their
partner does not reciprocate their feelings. This leads to frequent
breakups, often because the relationship feels cold and distant.
These individuals feel especially distraught after the end of a
relationship. Cassidy and Berlin described another pathological
pattern where ambivalently attached adults cling to young children
as a source of security (1994).
Avoidant Attachment
characteristics of avoidant attachment | |
as children | as adults |
may avoid parents | have difficulties with intimacy |
does not seek much comfort or contact from parents | invest little emotion in relationships |
shows no preference between parent and stranger. | unable tp share thoughts and feelings with others. |
Children with avoidant attachment styles tend to avoid parents
and caregivers. This avoidance often becomes especially pronounced
after a period of absence. These children might not reject
attention from a parent, but neither do they seek our comfort or
contact. Children with an avoidant attachment show no preference
between a parent and a complete stranger.
As adults, those with an avoidant attachment tend to have
difficulty with intimacy and close relationships. These individuals
do not invest much emotion in relationships and experience little
distress when a relationship ends. They often avoid intimacy by
using excuses (such as long work hours), or may fantasize about
other people during sex. Research has also shown that adults with
an avoidant attachment style are more accepting and likely to
engage in casual sex (Feeney, J., Noller, and Patty 1993). Other
common characteristics include a failure to support partners during
stressful times and an inability to share feelings, thoughts, and
emotions with partners.
Disorganized Attachment
characteristics of disorganized attachment | |
at age 1 | at age 6 |
show a mixture of resistant and avoidant behaviors | take on a parental role. |
seem confused, dazed, or apprehensive. | some children act as a caregiver toward the parent. |
Children with a disorganized-insecure attachment style show a
lack of clear attachment behavior. Their actions and responses to
caregivers are often a mix of behaviors, including avoidance or
resistance. These children are described as displaying dazed
behavior, sometimes seeming either confused or apprehensive in the
presence of a caregiver. Main and Solomon (1986) proposed that
inconsistent behavior on the part of parents might be a
contributing factor in this style of attachment. In later research,
Main and Hesse (1990) argued that parents who act as figures of
both fear and reassurance to a child contribute to a disorganized
attachment style. Because the child feels both comforted and
frightened by the parent, confusion results.
References
- Ainsworth, M.; Blehar, M.; Waters, E.; and Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of Attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and Loss, Vol. 1: Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
- Bowlby, J. (1979). The Making and Breaking of Affectional Bonds. London: Tavistock.
- Bowlby, J. (1988). A Secure Base. New York: Basic Books.
- Feeney, J. A.; Noller, P.; and Patty, J. (1993). "Adolescents\' Interactions with the Opposite Sex: Influence of Attachment Style and Gender." Journal of Adolescence 16, 169–186.
- Hazen, C. & Shaver, P. (1987) Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 511-524.
- Main, M., & Hesse, E. (1990). Parents\' unresolved traumatic experiences are related to infant disorganized attachment status: Is frightened/frightening parental behavior the linking mechanism? In M. T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E. M. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the Preschool Years: Theory, Research, and Intervention, 161-182. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
- Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1986). Discovery of an insecure-disorganized/ disoriented attachment pattern: Procedures, findings and implications for the classification of behavior. In T. B. Brazelton & M. Yogman (Eds.), Affective Development in Infancy, 95-124. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
- Mccarthy, G. (1999) Attachment style and adult love relationships and friendships: A study of a group of women at risk of experiencing relationship difficulties. British Journal of Medical Psychology, Volume 72, Number 3, September 1999, pp. 305-321(17).