Hypnosis: Medical Tool or Illusion?
A The image most people have of the mysterious
art of hypnotism is of a stage trick. But hypnotists are much more
likely nowadays to be scientists seeking ways to probe the
subconscious mind, or find a new way to relieve pain. But is
hypnosis a real phenomenon? If so, what is it useful for? Over the
past few years, researchers have found that hypnotized individuals
actively respond to suggestions even though they sometimes perceive
the dramatic changes in thought and behavior they experience as
happening "by themselves." During hypnosis, it is as though the
brain temporarily suspends its attempts to authenticate incoming
sensory information. Some people are more hypnotizable than others,
although scientists still don't know why. To study any phenomenon
properly, researchers must first have a way to measure it. In the
case of hypnosis, that yardstick is the Stanford Hypnotic
Susceptibility Scales. The Stanford scales, as they are often
called, were devised in the late 1950s by Stanford University
psychologists. One version of the Stanford scales consists of a
series of 12 activities—such as holding one's arm outstretched or
sniffing the contents of a bottle—that test the depth of the
hypnotic state. In the first instance, individuals are told that
they are holding a very heavy ball, and they are scored as
"passing" that suggestion if their arm sags under the imagined
weight. In the second case, subjects are told that they have no
sense of smell, and then a vial of ammonia is waved under their
nose. If they have no reaction, they are deemed very responsive to
hypnosis; if they grimace and recoil, they are not.
B Researchers with very different theoretical
perspectives now agree on several fundamental principles of
hypnosis. The first is that a person's ability to respond to
hypnosis is remarkably stable during adulthood. In addition, a
person's responsiveness to hypnosis also remains fairly consistent
regardless of the characteristics of the hypnotist: the
practitioner's gender, age and experience have little or no effect
on a subject's ability to be hypnotized. Similarly, the success of
hypnosis does not depend on whether a subject is highly motivated
or especially willing. A very responsive subject will become
hypnotized under a variety of experimental conditions and
therapeutic settings, whereas a less susceptible person will not,
despite his or her sincere efforts. (Negative attitudes and
expectations can, however, interfere with hypnosis.)
C Under hypnosis, subjects do not behave as
passive automatons but instead are active problem solvers who
incorporate their moral and cultural ideas into their behavior
while remaining exquisitely responsive to the expectations
expressed by the experimenter. Nevertheless, the subject does not
experience hypnotically suggested behavior as something that is
actively achieved. To the contrary, it is typically deemed as
effortless—as something that just happens. People who have been
hypnotized often say things like "My hand became heavy and moved
down by itself" or "Suddenly I found myself feeling no pain." Many
researchers now believe that these types of disconnections are at
the heart of hypnosis. In response to suggestion, subjects make
movements without conscious intent, fail to detect exceedingly
painful stimulation or temporarily forget a familiar fact. Of
course, these kinds of things also happen outside
hypnosis—occasionally in day-to-day life and more dramatically in
certain psychiatric and neurological disorders.
D Scientists think that hypnosis may relieve
pain by decreasing the activity of brain areas involved in the
experience of suffering. Positron emission tomography (PET) scans
of horizontal and vertical brain sections were taken while the
hands of hypnotized volunteers were dunked into painfully hot
water. The activity of the somatosensory cortex, which processes
physical stimuli, did not differ whether a subject was given the
hypnotic suggestion that the sensation would be painfully hot or
that it would be minimally unpleasant. In contrast, a part of the
brain known to be involved in the suffering aspect of pain, the
anterior cingulate cortex, was much less active when subjects were
told that the pain would be minimally unpleasant.
E Perhaps nowhere has hypnosis engendered more
controversy than over the issue of "recovered" memory. Cognitive
science has established that people are fairly adept at discerning
whether an event actually occurred or whether they only imagined
it. But under some circumstances, we falter. We can come to believe
(or can be led to believe) that something happened to us when, in
fact, it did not. One of the key cues humans appear to use in
making the distinction between reality and imagination is the
experience of effort. Apparently, at the time of encoding a memory,
a "tag" cues us as to the amount of effort we expended: if the
event is tagged as having involved a good deal of mental effort on
our part, we tend to interpret it as something we imagined. If it
is tagged as having involved relatively little mental effort, we
tend to interpret it as something that actually happened to us.
Given that the calling card of hypnosis is precisely the feeling of
effortlessness, we can see why hypnotized people can so easily
mistake an imagined past event for something that happened long
ago. Hence, something that is merely imagined can become ingrained
as an episode in our life story.
F So what are the medical benefits of
hypnosis? A 1996 National Institutes of Health technology
assessment panel judged hypnosis to be an effective intervention
for alleviating pain from cancer and other chronic conditions.
Voluminous clinical studies also indicate that hypnosis can reduce
the acute pain experienced by patients undergoing burn-wound
debridement, children enduring bone marrow aspirations and women in
labor. The pain-relieving effect of hypnosis is often substantial,
and in a few cases the degree of relief matches or exceeds that
provided by morphine. Hypnosis can boost the effectiveness of
psychotherapy for disorders such as obesity, insomnia, anxiety and
hypertension.
加载中,请稍候......