Does play help
develop bigger, better brains? Bryant Furlow
investigates
A.
Playing is a
serious business. Children engrossed in a make-believe world, fox
cubs play-fighting or kittens teaming a ball of string aren’t just
having fun. Play may look like a carefree and exuberant way to pass
the time before the hard work of adulthood comes along, but there’s
much more to it than that. For a start, play can even cost animals
their lives. Eighty percent of deaths among juvenile fur seals
occur because playing pups fail to sport predators approaching. It
is also extremely expensive in terms of energy. Playful young
animals use around two or three per cent of energy cavorting, and
in children that figure can be closer to fifteen per cent. ‘Even
two or three per cent is huge,’ says John Byers of Idaho
University. ‘You just don’t find animals wasting energy like that,’
he adds. There must be a
reason.
B.
But if play is
not simply a developmental hiccup, as biologists once thought, why
did it evolve? The latest idea suggests that play has evolved to
build big brains. In other words, playing makes you intelligent.
Playfulness, it seems, is common only among mammals, although a few
of the larger-brained birds also indulge. Animals at play often use
unique signs – tail-wagging in dogs, for example – to indicate that
activity superficially resembling adult behavior is not really in
earnest. In popular explanation of play has been that it helps
juveniles develop the skills they will need to hunt, mate and
socialise as adults. Another has been that it allows young animals
to get in shape for adult life by improving their respiratory
endurance. Both these ideas have been questioned in recent
years.
C.
Take the
exercise theory. If play evolved to build muscle or as a kind of
endurance training, then you would expect to see permanent
benefits. But Byers points out that the benefits of increased
exercise disappear rapidly after training stops, so many
improvement in endurance resulting from juvenile play would be lost
by adulthood. ‘If the function of play was to get into shape,’ says
Byers, ‘the optimum time for playing would depend on when it was
most advantageous for the young of a particular species to do so.
But it doesn’t work like that.’ Across species, play tends to peak
about halfway through the suckling stage and then
decline.
D.
Then there’s the
skills- training hypothesis. At first glance, playing animals do
appear to be practising the complex manoeuvres they will need in
adulthood. But a closer inspection reveals this interpretation as
too simplistic. In one study, behavioural ecologist Tim Caro, from
the
University of California, looked at the predatory play of kittens
and their predatory behaviour when they reached adulthood. He found
that the way the cats played had no significant effect on their
hunting prowess in later
life.
E.
Earlier this
year, Sergio Pellis of Lethbridge University, Canada, reported that
there is a strong positive link between brain size and playfulness
among mammals in general. Comparing measurements for fifteen orders
of mammals, he and his team found large brains (for a given body
size) are linked to greater playfulness. The converse was also
found to be true. Robert Barton of Durham University believes that,
because large brains are more sensitive to developmental stimuli
than smaller brains, they require more play to help mould them for
adulthood. ‘I concluded it’s to do with learning, and with the
importance of environmental data to the brain during development,’
he says.
F.
According to
Byers, the timing of the playful stage in young animals provides an
important clue to what’s going on. If you plot the amount of time
juvenile devotes to play each day over the course of its
development, you discover a pattern typically associated with a
‘sensitive period’ – a brief development window during which the
brain can actually be modified in ways that are not possible
earlier or later in life. Think of the relative ease with which
young children – but not infants or adults – absorb language. Other
researchers have found that play in cats, rats and mice is at its
most intense just as this ‘window of opportunity” reaches its
peak.
G.
‘People have not
paid enough attention to the amount of the brain activated by
plays,’ says Marc Bekoff from Colorado University. Bekoff studied
coyote pups at play and found that the kind of behaviour involved
was markedly more variable and unpredictable than that of adults.
Such behaviour activates many different parts of the brain, he
reasons. Bekoff likens it to a behavioural kaleidoscope, with
animals at play jumping rapidly between activities. ‘They use
behaviour from a lot of different contexts – predation, aggression,
reproduction,’ he says. ‘Their developing brain is getting all
sorts of
stimulation.’
H.
Not only is more
of the brain involved in play that was suspected, but it also seems
to activate higher cognitive processes. ‘There’s enormous cognitive
involvement in play,’ says Bekoff. He points out that play often
involves complex assessments of playmates, ideas of reciprocity and
the use of specialised signals and rules. He believes that play
creates a brain that has greater behavioural flexibility and
improved potential for learning later in life. The idea is backed
up by the work of Stephen Siviy of Gettysburg College. Siviy
studied how bouts of play affected the brain’s levels of particular
chemical associated with the stimulation and growth of nerve cells.
He was surprised by the extent of the activation. ‘Play just lights
everything up,’ he says. By allowing link-ups between brain areas
that might not normally communicate with each other, play may
enhance creativity.
I.
What might
further experimentation suggest about the way children are raised
in many societies today? We already know that rat pups denied the
chance to play grow smaller brain components and fail to develop
the ability to apply social rules when they interact with their
peers. With schooling beginning earlier and becoming increasingly
exam-orientated, play is likely to get even less of a look-in. Who
knows what the result of that will
be?