分类: 工作生活 |
未经许可请勿转载
Why do we
sleep?
human requires about 8-hours of sleep every day, which amounts to
56
hours per week, 240 hours per month and 2,920 hours per year!
Indeed,
one third of our lives is spent in a state of relative
unconsciousness
and inactivity! Everyone (especially the college students)
has
occasionally wondered why we might need such a thing as sleep.
Yes,
North American college students must hate sleep. They don't go to
bed
until 3 or 4 a.m. in the morning thinking that sleeping is a
complete
waste of time that could be better spent partying or watching TV
(or
perhaps studying...I guess), but is sleep really nothing but a
needless
distraction? Even though you may want to spend less time on sleep,
you
still understand that sleep is necessary for your survival.
Furthermore, though it appears that the body enters a
relatively
motionless and seemingly unconscious state during sleep, the brain
is
actually very active. This seems to be somewhat counter-intuitive
on
the surface.
sufficient to define sleep as a lack of consciousness, since it
is
clear from brain activity patterns that being unconscious is
different
from being asleep. A common way scientists record brain activity is
by
attaching electrodes to the scalp of a subject. The electrodes
are
then connected to a machine called an "electroencephalograph",
which
produces the wavy lines of an electroencephalogram (EEG) to
illustrate
what most people know as "brain waves". In the modern age, sleep
is
defined in the context of EEG patterns and from numerous
studies,
scientists have found that, contrary to common belief, sleep is not
a
single, uniform state but instead can be divided into two
distinct
phases: rapid-eye-movement (REM) sleep and non-REM (NREM)
sleep.
Human adults generally complete the two stages in 90 minutes or 4-5
times
during an 8-hour period of sleep.
frequency synchronous waves. This phase can be further divided
into
four sub-stages (Stage 1, Stage 2, Stage 3 and Stage 4) according
to
their EEG patterns. Stages 3 and 4 are sometimes called slow
wave
sleep (SWS), during which the frequency of the brain waves is
less
than 2 Hz. Low EEG frequencies do not necessarily mean that
the
neurons in the sleeping brain fire less, but rather, that neurons
are
firing in a highly synchronized fashion. REM sleep is a
relatively
deep stage of sleep and is characterized by the presence of rapid
eye
movements as its name indicates. During this stage, low-voltage
and
high-frequency waves become prominent in EEG recordings.
Surprisingly,
the REM EEG looks almost identical to waking EEG patterns. Most
dream
reports have come from awakenings during this stage. The REM
sleep
stage has consequently been associated with dream production for
a
long time. The story is not quite that simple, however, since
dreaming
occurs during NREM sleep to a lesser degree and are not
easily
recalled. Interestingly, during REM sleep, the muscles of the
entire
body expect those controlling eye movement and respiratory system
are
completely paralyzed, which prevents us from acting out our
dreams.
There are, unfortunately, some degenerative brain diseases in
the
elderly that can cause aberrant REM sleep behavior disorders. Due
to
their inability to inhibit the expression of motor actions in
REM
sleep, these people may literally act out their dream scenarios,
which
may lead to tragic outcomes. In summary, awakeness, NREM sleep and
REM
sleep are the three distinct states of brain function accompanied
by
large shifts in body functions.
although scientists do have quite a few "theories". The 3
basic
theories are: 1) sleep is an adaptive process, 2) sleep has a
restorative function and 3) sleep plays a role in learning and
memory
consolidation. According to the first theory, some have argued that
we
do not really require sleep at all. It is an inherited
adaptive
behavior that was a result of evolution. Sleep may have
developed
because of a need of animals to protect themselves. For example,
some
animals search for food and water during the day because it is
easier
to see when the sun is out. When it gets dark, it is best for
these
animals to save energy to avoid getting eaten, and possibly
falling
off a cliff that they cannot see. When comparing sleeping
habits,
animals that serve as prey for predators sleep the least.
Conversely,
the adaptations linked to animal order appear to have
relatively
little to do with determining the length of REM sleep, as the
REM
sleep time can vary greatly even across close species. Sleep
surely
has many adaptive functions like conservation of energy, repair
of
injury, and defense from predation. But is that all?
process. That probably makes perfect sense to most people. You go
to
bed because you feel fatigue, and you feel fresh again after having
a
good sleep. The restoration theory suggests that sleep helps the
body
recover from all the work it did while the individual was
awake.
Experiments have shown that the more physical exercise an animal
does,
the more NREM the animal will have (but this is not true if
comparing
between different species?. Moreover, if people are deprived of
NREM
sleep by being awakened every time they get to stage 4 SWS,
the
majority complain of being physically tired. So, it appears that
sleep
not only conserves energy, but also restores energy. In fact, a
group
of researchers have shown that NREM sleep restores brain
glycogen
stores. A chemical called adenosine is also believed to play
an
important role in sleep. But in contrast to the energy molecules
such
as glycogen that is being restored during sleep, our brain is
trying
to get rid of exceeding amount of adenosine during sleep. The role
of
adenosine became clear when scientists tried to figure out
how
caffeine works to keep us stay alert. Researchers once believed
that
the chemical blocked the same sedating mechanisms that
tranquilizers
induced but soon scientists found that caffeine actually blocked
the
receptors (i.e. the special receiving areas on cells) of adenosine
and
thus prevented its effect. Now, increasing evidence is confirming
that
adenosine is indeed an important factor that induces fatigue
and
sleepiness. Adenosine originally comes from a large energy
molecule
known as adenosine triphosphate (ATP). As ATP loses chemical
groups
known as phosphates, energy is released from the molecule to
fuel
billions of other reactions that take place in the cells when we
are
awake. Finally, ATP is dwindled down to adenosine, which then
gets
accumulated in the brain as a result of prolonged
wakefulness.
However, we don't know yet how the brain reduces the concentration
of
adenosine during sleep. Some researchers are now attempting
to
determine whether sleep relates backs to ATP by examining the
relationship between adenosine and ATP on a cellular level.
be the sole purposes of sleep. Surprisingly, an increasing amount
of
research in both animals and humans has suggested that sleep also
aids
in securing memories and helps learning. Actually, the idea that
sleep
is important for learning and memory has been hotly debated for
years.
Recent advances in neuroscience research have allowed scientists
to
conduct a variety of careful studies in both animals and humans,
which
have provided clear evidence that sleep is indeed important for
at
least some types of memory and learning. Now it is believed that
REM
sleep is especially important for procedural learning (i.e.
the
learning of a motor skill or action performance such as riding
a
bicycle or playing a melody on the piano), whereas slow wave sleep
has
been shown to be of greater importance in the consolidation
of
explicit or declarative memory, which involves recall of
specific
events or knowledge (such as the memory of what you've eaten
for
today's lunch or knowing who's the most stupid US president). It
has
also been reported that both REM and SWS are required for the
consolidation and processing of emotional memories.
straightforward task. Some sleep-memory studies have relied on a
sleep
deprivation paradigm, which obviously had a great number of
secondary
effects on attention, disposition and motivation that may
have
complicated data interpretation. Nevertheless, there are a number
of
intriguing observations consistent with a role of sleep in
memory
consolidation. For example, in one human study, participants had
to
perform the simple motor task of repeatedly typing a sequence on
a
keyboard. The results showed that a full night's sleep after
training
improved their performance by almost 20 percent, compared to
the
performance before sleep. Another study also has shown that sleep
is
required immediately following learning since participants who
were
sleep deprived the night following training but were allowed
two
subsequent nights of recovery sleep showed no significant
task
improvement.
reprocess newly learned information so that memories can stick
longer.
This idea is supported by the results from the studies using
a
technology know as brain imaging to map brain activity.
Several
research groups have found that the brain areas that were
activated
during procedural task training were re-activated during REM
sleep.
Furthermore, during REM sleep, the brain areas that showed
significant
activation during task performance were more active in
trained
subjects than in untrained subjects. In other words, it appears
that
our brain "replays" learned experiences while asleep, probably
as
part of a process of memory consolidation. In addition, there are
a
number of correlative studies suggesting that the loss of these
of
"replay" episodes could lead to memory
deficits.
from literature that are against the idea of the necessity of
sleep
per se in memory consolidation. For example, patients with
specific
types of brain injury or on certain types of medication can
have
profound disruptions in sleep patterns or do not sleep at all.
There
are also a few examples of individuals who spontaneously
become
insomniac for their entire remaining lifetime. But these phenomena
do
not lead to any profound memory disruption in these
individuals,
apparently dissociating sleep from memory formation. Given
the
ambiguity in defining sleep in these reports, however, it is
certainly
possible that these insomniac individuals may still have certain
sleep-
like patterns of EEG activity that may serve as functional
substitutes
of sleep. For the time being, no one really knows.
in learning and memory consolidation. At this point, however,
the evidence provides enough arguments that it's time for
undergraduates to stop staying up all night and catch some
zzzz's,
especially before exams. And for those who need an excuse for
getting
some extra sleep, just say, "I'm not sleeping; I'm learning."
K.Fan